Contents
Linguistic classification
- Niger-Congo
- Atlantic-Congo
- Mabia
- Mabia Central
- Mabia Mid-Central
- Mabia Buli-Konni
- Buli
- Mabia Buli-Konni
- Mabia Mid-Central
- Mabia Central
- Mabia
- Atlantic-Congo
Tone system
There are three tonal levels in Buli, i.e. high, mid and low, and the tone phenomena are very complex. Words deviate from their basic tonal patterns when they occur in a syntactic schema.
Lexical function
(1) | a. |
bāng |
'bangle’ | ||
b. |
bàng | |
'lizard'2 |
Grammatical functions
(2) | a. | Ateng | a | chēng |
yabanga. |
'Ateng is going to the market.' | |||||
b. | Ateng | a | chèng |
yabanga. | |
'Ateng was going to the market.'2 |
Noun class system2
There are four singular classes and five plural classes. While the singular class identifiers are not markers of number, the plural markers mark number. This classification is based on semantics rather than morphology. Items in class one are [+human ] and the suffixes do not mark number, but they mostly serve as determiners. The items in all other classes are [-human] and their plural suffixes mark number.
Class | Singular | Plural | Example | Semantics |
I | wà | bà | núr 'man' | human / loan words |
II | dì | ŋà | yérí 'house' | dependent entities (body parts, fruits, languages) |
III | kà | sì | bàŋ 'lizard' | ethnonyms, trees, diminutives |
IV | kù | tì | síuk 'path' | animals, instruments, mass and abstract nouns |
V | bù | - | trees, body parts, animals, liquids, abstract nouns |
Pronominal System1
Personal Pronouns / Possessive Pronouns
In Buli, there is a distinction between speaker, hearer and topic. The speaker corresponds to the first person, while the hearer is second person. Topic on the other hand refers to the third person, but is treated differently from first and second person in that the third person form corresponds to a nominal class pronoun. In contrast, the first and second person pronouns are formed by a strong or weak person pronoun form. In general, these pronouns show number agreement. Moreover, the topic forms not only express person and number, but also gives rise to the differentiation between the five singular and four plural forms of the noun class system1.
According to Sulemana (2021), there is a distinction between strong and deficient (weak) pronouns which differ in their tonal appearance. The strong forms have a high tone, whereas the deficient forms have a low tone. An overview of the inventory of the personal pronouns in Buli is given in the table below.
Pers/Num/CL | Strong forms | Deficient (weak) forms | ||
1SG | mí | ǹ (mə) | ||
2SG | fí | fì (fə) | ||
3SG.CL1 | wá | wà | ||
3SG.CL2 | dí | dì | ||
3SG.CL3 | ká | kà | ||
3SG.CL4 | kú | kù | ||
3SG.CL5 | bú | bù | ||
1PL | támà | tì | ||
2PL | námà | nì | ||
3PL.CL1 | bá | bà | ||
3PL.CL2 | sí | sì | ||
3PL.CL3 | tí | tì | ||
3PL.CL4 | ŋá | ŋà |
Interestingly, for the first and second person pronouns in singular there are three different forms. The pronouns in deficient form mə and fə can only appear in object function shown in (3a) and (3b), while the pronouns ǹ and fì can only serve as subjects, shown in (3c) and (3d). The pronouns in strong form on the other hand can appear in both subject and object position. This is shown in (3e) and (3f). Furthermore, the person pronouns shown in the table above can also be used as possessive pronouns, meaning that they are homonymous. An example is given in (4), in which the the possessive pronoun can either appear as an independent element as in (4a) or it can be clitically bound with the noun as in (4b).
(3) | a. | Asouk | nàgì |
mə/fə. |
Asouk | hit |
1SG/2SG | ||
'Asouk hit me/you.’ | ||||
b. | *Mə/fə | nágí |
Asouk. | |
1SG/2SG | hit |
Asouk | ||
'I/you hit Asouk.' | ||||
c. | Ǹ | nàgí |
Asouk. | |
1SG | hit |
Asouk | ||
'I hit Asouk.' | ||||
d. | *Asouk | nàgì |
ǹ. | |
Asouk | hit |
1SG | ||
'Asouk hit me.' | ||||
e. | Mí | nágí |
Asouk. | |
1SG | hit |
Asouk | ||
'I hit Asouk.' | ||||
f. | Asouk | nàgì |
mí. | |
Asouk | hit |
1SG | ||
'Asouk hit me.'3 |
(4) | a. | Mí | bìmbìlī | lē |
ǹnā. |
1.SG | pot | CONJ |
DEM:DET | ||
'This is my pot.’ | |||||
b. | M=bìmbìlī | lē |
ǹnā. | ||
1.SG=pot | CONJ |
DEM:DET | |||
'This is my pot.'1 |
Absolute Pronouns
Pronouns that refer to entities, independent of a verbal predicate, always have to appear in the strong form. In these contexts, there is no possible option of cliticalization, because the focus marker ká as the only preceding element does not obligatorily need to be present. The following examples illustrate this fact.
(5) | a. | (Ká) |
mí. |
FOC |
1.SG | ||
'It's me.’ | |||
b. | *(Ká) |
ǹ. | |
FOC |
1.SG | ||
'It's me.'1 |
(6) | a. | (Ká) |
fí. |
FOC |
2.SG | ||
'It's you.’ | |||
b. | *(Ká) |
fì. | |
FOC |
2.SG | ||
'It's you.'1 |
(7) | a. | (Ká) |
wá. |
FOC |
1.PL | ||
'It's us.’ | |||
b. | *(Ká) |
wà. | |
FOC |
1.PL | ||
'It's us.'1 |
Reflexive Pronouns
In order to express reflexivity in Buli, an additional particle dék is used, which can either follow the strong form of a pronoun or it can clitically bind with the deficient form of a pronoun. The strong form is used in order to express logophoric reference, as shown in (9), while the deficient form is used in cases, in which the agent and the patient of a predicate are coreferent. This is shown in (8a). An overview of the reflexive pronouns is given in the table below.1
Pers/Num/CL | Strong forms | Deficient (weak) forms | ||
1SG | mí dék | ǹ=dēk | ||
2SG | fí dék | fì=dēk | ||
3SG.CL1 | wá dék | wà=dēk | ||
3SG.CL2 | dí dék | dì=dēk | ||
3SG.CL3 | ká dék | kà=dēk | ||
3SG.CL4 | kú dék | kù=dēk | ||
3SG.CL5 | bú dék | bù=dēk | ||
1PL | tàmā dék | tì=dēk | ||
2PL | nàmā dék | nì=dēk | ||
3PL.CL1 | bá dék | bà=dēk | ||
3PL.CL2 | sí dék | sì=dēk | ||
3PL.CL3 | tí dék | tì=dēk | ||
3PL.CL4 | ŋá dék | ŋà=dēk |
(8) | a. | Mí | nág |
ǹ=dēk | ||
1SG | hit |
1SG=REFL | ||||
'I hit myself.’ | ||||||
b. | Mí | zágí | mí | dék |
yìtìyā. | |
1SG | rise | 1SG | REFL |
get.up | ||
'I got up (myself).'1 |
(9) | Bà=wèènì | àyēn | bá | dék | lè |
jām. |
3PL.CL1=say | COMP | 3PL.CL1 | REFL | FUT |
come | |
'They said that they themselves will come.’1 |
Reciprocal Pronouns
The particle dék expressing reflexivity in a construction with a pronoun can also be used in reciprocal contexts, shown in (10). In order to disambiguate the expression, the reciprocal nominal element chāāb can be used in object position. This is shown in (11).
(10) | Bà=nàg | ká |
bà=dēk. |
3PL.CL1=hit | FOC |
3PL.CL1=REFL | |
'They hit themselves / each other.’1 |
(11) | Tì=ɲà |
chāāb. |
1PL=see |
REZ | |
'We saw each of us. ’1 |
Indefinite Pronouns
Indefinite pronouns are formed by the stem of the pronouns of the noun classes (3SG.CL1-5 and 3.PL.CL1-4) with an additional vowel. In contexts involving negation or conditionals, these pronouns refer to NPI elements like nobody, no one. 1
CL | SG | PL | ||
1 | wāā(i) | bāā(i) | ||
2 | dīī | sīī | ||
3 | kāā(i) | tīī | ||
4 | kūū(i) | ŋāā(i) | ||
5 | būū(i) |
Demonstrative Pronouns
In general, there are two demonstrative pronouns in Buli, dɛ, lá.
Dɛ
The demonstrative form dɛ is used in contexts, in which an entity to which the pronoun refers to is visible. The form itself can be translated to here. The pronoun is adjoined to the definite noun as a suffix, as illustrated in (12).
(12) | Bà=bòrà=ā | nāg | gɔgtàŋā, | máástàwàdɛ | nē | dèrì |
tààm ... |
NC1.PL=LOC.be=there=IPF | hit | dance.PL:DEF | master:DEF:there | CONJ | immediately |
pass | |
'While they drummed to the dance, the teacher immediately came ... .’1 |
For this demonstrative form there exists also a second more complex form, actually. The basis of the form comes from a noun class pronoun, to which the demonstrative is adjoined as a suffix and the morphem ɲā is optionally prepended. An overview of the demonstrative noun class pronouns formed with dɛ is given below.1
CL | SG | PL | ||
1 | (ɲā) wādɛ | (ŋā) bādɛ | ||
2 | (ɲā) dīdɛ | (ŋā) sīdɛ | ||
3 | (ɲā) kādɛ | (ŋā) tīdɛ | ||
4 | (ɲā) kūdɛ | (ŋā) ŋādɛ | ||
5 | (ɲā) būdɛ |
Lá
In contexts, in which the entity to which the demonstrative refers to is not visible, the form lá is used. This demonstrative form is also formed by a noun class pronoun, to which the demonstrative also adjoins as a suffix. This demonstrative can be translated to that. An overview of these forms is given in the table below and an example for a context in which the demonstrative lá is used is given in (13).
CL | SG | PL | ||
1 | wálá | bálá | ||
2 | dílá | sílá | ||
3 | kálá | tílá | ||
4 | kúlá | ŋálá | ||
5 | búlá |
The demonstrative can also form a compound with a preceding noun, as shown in (13b).
(13) | a. | Bà=nē | ɲɛ | dílá |
nùè=lā ... | |||
NC1.PL=CONJ | do | CL2.SG:DET |
finish=DET | |||||
'After they finished that, ... .’ | ||||||||
b. | Mí | bísáŋá | à | yāā | ká | yí-dílá |
yīkā. | |
1SG | child.PL:DEF | IPF | like | FOC | song-NC2.SG:DET |
sing.N | ||
'My children love to sing this song. '1 |
Interrogative Pronouns
Which
The interrogative pronoun for which is formed by the pronoun form of the noun classes with the suffix -nà.1
CL | SG | PL | ||
1 | wànà | bànà | ||
2 | dìnà | sìnà | ||
3 | kànà | tìnà | ||
4 | kùnà | ŋànà | ||
5 | bùnà |
Who
The singular and plural forms of the first noun class interrogative pronouns (which) are used as the question word for human referents (who). Note that the abbreviation INT denotes interrogativity.
(14) | Ká |
wànàà? |
FOC |
CL1.SG.which.INT | |
'Who is that?.’1 |
How much/many
In order to express how much/many, the interrogative form of the second class singular dìnà is used. It can either appear with the focus marker only or preceded by a substantive as antecedent.1
(15) | a. | Ká |
dìnàà? | ||
FOC |
how.much.INT | ||||
'How much/many?’ | |||||
b. | Jà-nàlìŋkàdɛ | à | da=ká |
dìnàà? | |
Thing-pretty:DEF:here | & | sell=FOC |
how.much.INT | ||
'How much is this pretty thing?'1 |
The interrogative form can also combine with the strong form of the personal pronouns from the noun classes in order to function as an adnominal quantifier of a preceding noun.
(16) | Nídɔābā | bà=dìnà | àlē |
jàmìyāā? |
man.PL | CL.1.PL=how.many | &:CONJ |
come:ASS.INT | |
'How many men came?'1 |
What
The interrogative form for what is bɔà, which can either form a compound like in (15a), or which modifies a following noun, as shown in (15b).
(17) | a. | Ká | yí-bɔà | àtè | fàà | wōŋ |
fì=yīì? | |
FOC | song-what | &:CONJ | 2.SG:IPF | say.COMP |
2SG=sing.INT | |||
'What (which song) are you going to sing?' | ||||||||
b. | Ká | bɔàn | yìīlì | àtè | fàà | wōŋ |
fì=yīì? | |
FOC | what | song | &:CONJ | 2SG:IPF | say.COMP |
2SG=sing.INT | ||
'What (which song) are you going to sing?' 1 |
How
The interrogative pronoun for how is sɛ`, that appears in a position preceded by the focus marker ká.
(18) | Fí | ɲɛ=kú | ká |
sɛ? |
2SG | do=CL.4SG | FOC |
how.INT | |
'How did you do it?’1 |
Where
There are two local question words in Buli, lèē and bɛɛ. The former relates to locations of entities like in (17a), while the latter is used adverbally like in (17b).
(19) | a. | Fì=yènní |
lèē? | ||
2SG=house:DEF |
where.INT | ||||
'Where is your house?’ | |||||
b. | Fàà | chēŋ | ká |
bɛɛ? | |
2SG:IPF | go | FOC |
where.INT | ||
'Where are you going (to)?'1 |
When
For the temporal interrogative pronoun there exist several forms in Buli. One form is a compound consisting of the noun tám ("time") and the question word for what. Another and more precise form like "which day/ month/...?" can be formed by the interrogative form for "which" and a noun like "day", as dà-dìnàà. Alternatively, there is a third form dìmpōɔ/dìsàpō, which refers to a more general point in time.
(20) | a. | Ká | tám-bɔà | tè |
wà=kpìì? |
FOC | time-what | CONJ |
CL.1SG=die.INT | ||
'When did he die?' | |||||
b. | ɔ=jàm | ká |
dà-dìnàà? | ||
CL.1SG=come | FOC |
day-CL.2SG.which.INT | |||
'When did he come?' | |||||
c. | Wà=kpìì | ká |
dìmpōɔ? | ||
CL.1SG=die.INT | FOC |
when.INT | |||
'When did he die?'1 |
Word order
Buli has a strict SVO word order with optional focus/wh-movement and no pro-drop. In (21) an intransitive clause with an adverb is given, in (22) a transitive clause also with an adverb is shown and in (23) the word order in an embedded clause is shown. All examples confirm the basic word order of SVO.
(21) | Mí | ɲú=ká |
ɲwūlì. |
1.SG | drink=FOC |
quickly | |
'I drank quickly.’1 |
(22) | Núrúwá | kàlì | mɔātī | (ká) |
fì. |
Person:DEF | sit | himself.next | (FOC) |
2.SG | |
'The man lives next to you.’1 |
(23) | Asibi | wìen | āyīn | Asouk | dìgì |
làmmú. |
Asibi | say | COMP | Asouk | cook |
meat.DEF | |
'Asibi said that Asouk cooked the meat.’3 |
Verbal System
The verbal system in Buli is characterized by tonal inflection and relatively simple segmental verb morphology. Most of the verbs have a single segmental basic form, to which either a preverbal or postverbal particle is added and a specific tone in order to mark different aspects, modes, affirmation as well as negation.1
Preverbal Particles
Preverbal particles mainly mark aspect and polarity. Note that the absence of a preverbal particle indicates perfective aspect.
Á, À
The preverbal particle á, à marks imperfective aspect. In the subjunctive the particle á is used, while its counterpart with low tone à is used in the indicative. In (24a) this particle thus occurs in subjunctive and in (24b) in indicative.
(24) | a. | Núrúmá | á | bōb |
nííŋá. |
Person.PL:DEF | IPFV | tie |
cow.PL:DEF | ||
|
'The people should tie the cows (again).’ | ||||
b. | Núrúmá | à | bōb |
nííŋá. | |
Person.PL:DEF | IPFV | tie |
cow.PL:DEF | ||
'The people tie the cows (again).'1 |
Lè
The preverbal particle lè marks future tense. It can either attach to a preceding pronoun as in (25a) or it can occur as an independent particle as in (25b).
(25) | a. | Tì=lè | wēēn |
núrú-mbàlá. |
1PL=FUT | say |
Person-DEM:3PL.CL1:DET | ||
'We will inform the other people.’ | ||||
b. | Tììmū | lè |
bōlisi. | |
tree:DEF | FUT |
cut | ||
' The tree will be cut. '1 |
Kán, kàn
This preverbal particle kán, kàn is a preverbal negative marker. Similarly to the tone pattern of the preverbal particle á, à, in the subjunctive the preverbal particle kán has a high tone on the vowel, while in the indicative it has a low tone kàn.
(26) | Ǹ=kàn |
bàntí=fùʔ. |
1SG=NEG1 |
say.goodbye=2SG.NEG2 | |
'I will not say goodbye to you.’1 |
Main Verb
Postverbal Particles
Postverbal particles in Buli mainly express affirmation and negation.
Ya
The postverbal particle yā expresses assertion and is used in cases in which there is no preverbal particle, thus in perfective aspect. In contrast to the preverbal particles, this postverbal particle has to attach to the verb and cannot appear as an independent particle. The example in (27) illustrates that the speaker expresses a particularly surprising aspect of the facts. Moreover, predicates marked with this particle are used by speakers as an unexpected or unforeseeable change of situation, occasionally also adverbially translated as 'just now', 'immediately' or 'suddenly'.1
(27) | Bà=bòlìsì |
tììmū=yā. |
3PL.CL1=trim |
tree:DEF=ASS | |
'They trimmed the tree.’’1 |
Kámā
This particle expresses affirmation. Interestingly, the focus marker kà seems to be contained in the morphem ká-, while the morphem -mā seems to be an element with unknown function.1 The emphatic function of this particle is to establish a relation between a truth value of a proposition and an expression from the previous context of the discourse, (i.e. indeed, really). It seems that this particle expresses verum focus, but according to Schwarz (2005) it is an outstanding issue to investigate whether varying positions of the particle in the clause lead to semantic-pragmatic effects.
(28) | Mí | ɲá=wá |
kámā. |
1SG | see=3SG.CL1 |
AFF | |
'I really saw him.’1 |
Là
The postverbal particle là is formally identical with the demonstrative pronoun lá and also has an emphatic function. In Schwarz (2005), the use of this particle is described as the use of emphasis on the subject, which is characterized to a particular degree by the facts predicated on it as standing out from the crowd of potential alternatives. In (28) this particle stresses the property of being late and having a big nose.
(29) | a. |
Fì=bèní=là. | |
2SG=be.late=EMPH | |||
'You are really late.’ | |||
b. | Fì=ɲōnní |
zùà=là. | |
2SG=nose:DEF |
big=EMPH | ||
'Your nose is very big.'1 |
Negation
Negation in Buli is expressed by two negative markers, one occurs preverbally and the other one postverbally. The example in (30a) illustrates negation in the perfective, while (30b) illustrates it in the imperfective. In both examples, there is a preverbal negative marker and a postverbal one, similar to other negation systems like in French ne ... pas.
(30) | a. | Fí | àn | dìgi | lām |
ā. |
2SG | NEG1 | cook | meat |
NEG2 | ||
'You didn't cook meat.’ | ||||||
b. | Fí | kàn | dìgi | lām |
ā. | |
2SG | NEG1 | cook | meat |
NEG2 | ||
'You don't cook meat.'1 |
Preverbal negative markers
The following table gives an overview of the preverbal negative markers, that appear between the subject and the negated verb.4 The negative markers for the imperative II and the future tense differ only in tones, such that in the imperative II the tone on the vowel is low and the tone on the end is high, while in the future tense it is the exact opposite. The form of the indicative II is the form that deviates the most from the other forms. According to Schwarz (1999), the negative marker àn is associated with a verb in the perfective aspect, while the negative marker kàn is associated with imperfective aspect.
Tense | Buli | |||
Imperative I | kàá kūrī | |||
Imperative II | kàń kūrī | |||
Future | káǹ kūrī | |||
Indicative I | kàn kūrī | |||
Indicative II | àn kùríyà |
Postverbal negative markers
Contrary to preverbal negative markers, it is not obligatory for postverbal negative markers to show up.3 These rather stress the negated clause.
Postverbal Glottal Stop
All negated predicates in Buli have a hart glottal stop at the end of the clause. This glottal stop stresses the negation at the end of the clause and thus functions as a second negative marker. Note that this glottal stop is not always included in the glossing.
(31) | Wá=kàn | kpì |
dōnláʔ. |
3SG.CL1=NEG1 | die |
year.DEM.NEG2 | |
'He will not die this year.’4 |
(Y)ā
For reasons of completeness, the postverbal negative marker (y)ā is listed here as well. Note that the example in (30) thus is repeated here.
(32) | a. | Fí | àn | dìgi | lām |
ā. |
2SG | NEG1 | cook | meat |
NEG2 | ||
'You didn't cook meat.’ | ||||||
b. | Fí | kàn | digi | lām |
ā. | |
2SG | NEG1 | cook | meat |
NEG2 | ||
'You don't cook meat.'1 |
Question Formation5
In Buli, questions can be formed by an ex situ strategy as well as an in situ strategy. In addition, embedded questions and multiple questions are discussed as well.
Ex situ
Questions that exhibit the question word ex situ are formed by the order QVO, in which the particle ká can optionally precede the question word. Note that this particle is homonymous with the focus marker ká. In subject questions the particle ālì obligatorily follows the subject wh-phrase as in (33a), whereas in non-subject questions the particle ātì immediately follows the non-subject wh-phrase.
(33) | a. | (Ká) | wānā | *(ālì) | dìg |
lāmmúː? |
Q | who | PTC | cook |
meat.DEF | ||
'Who cooked the meat?’ | ||||||
b. | (Ká) | bwā | *(ātì) | bíːká |
dìgìː? | |
Q | what | PTC | child.DEF |
cook | ||
'What is it that the child cooked?'5 |
In situ
In questions, in which the question word occurs in situ, the particle ká obligatorily precedes the question word or the phrase containing the question word, respectively.
(34) | a. | Bíːká | dìg | *(ká) |
bwāː? | ||
child.DEF | cook | Q |
what | ||||
'What did the child cook?’ | |||||||
b. | Bíːká | tè | *(ká) | wānā |
lāmmúː? | ||
child.DEF | give | Q | who |
meat.DEF | |||
'Who did the child give the meat to?' | |||||||
c. | Bíːká | dìg | lāmmú | *(ká) |
bɛ̄ː? | ||
child.DEF | cook | meat.DEF | Q |
where | |||
'Where did the child cook the meat?' | |||||||
d. | Bíːká | dìg | lāmmú | tē | *(ká) |
wānā:? | |
child.DEF | cook | meat.DEF | give | Q |
who | ||
'Who did the child cook the meat for?'5 |
Embedded
In embedded questions the question word remains in situ and is embedded via the complementizer āsī, which only occurs in embedded contexts. In declarative contexts the complementizer āyīn is used.
(35) | a. | Mary | bèg | āsī | John | dìg | *(ká) |
bwāː. |
Mary | ask | C | John | cook | Q |
what | ||
'Mary asked what John has cooked.’ | ||||||||
b. | Mary | à-bā | āsī | John | dìg | *(ká) |
bwāː. | |
Mary | IPFV-wonder | C | John | cook | Q |
what | ||
'Mary wonders what John has cooked.'5 | ||||||||
Multiple questions
In ex situ, in situ and embedded questions it is possible to have more than one question word. In multiple questions, the particle ká precedes the highest wh-containing phrase.
(36) | a. | Ajohn | tè | ká | wān |
bwāː? |
Ajohn | give | Q | who |
what | ||
'Who did John give what?’ | ||||||
b. | (Ká) | wānā | ālì | dìg |
bwāː? | |
Q | who | PTC | cook |
what | ||
'Who cooked what? | ||||||
c. | (Ká) | bwā | ātì | wānā |
dìgìː? | |
Q | what | PTC | who |
cook | ||
'What is it that who cooks?'5 |
References
- 1. Schwarz, Anne (2005): Aspekte der Morphosyntax und Tonologie im Buli. Ph. D. thesis. Berlin: Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin
- 2. Bodomo, Adams, Hasiyatu Abubakari & Samuel Alhassan Issah (2020): Handbook of the Mabia Languages of West Africa. Glienicke: Galda Verlag
- 3. Sulemana, Abdul-Razak (2021): Non-finite Complementation: A case study of Bùlì. Ph. D. thesis.: Massachusetts Institute of Technology
- 4. Schwarz, Anne (1999): Preverbal negative markers in Buli .: Cahiers Voltaïques / Gur Papers 4, 91-98
- 5. Sulemana, Abdul-Razak (2019): Q-particles and the nature of covert movement: Evidence from Buli.: Glossa, 1-21
- 6. Abdul-Razak Sulemana. (2022): Obligatory controlled subjects in Bùlì. https://doi.org/10.5281/ZENODO.6393754.: Zenodo
- 7. Ferreira, Marcelo & Heejeong Ko (2003): Questions in Buli. In: Michael Kenstowicz & George Akanlig-Pare (eds.), Studies in Buli grammar: Working papers on endangered and less familiar languages 4. Cambridge, MA: MITWPL, pp. 35-44
- 8. Hiraiwa, Ken (2003): Relativization in Buli.: Working papers on Endangered and Less Familiar Languages 4, 45-84
- 9. Hiraiwa, Ken (2005): Predicate clefts in Buli: Phase and category.: Linguistic Analysis: a special volume: African linguistics in the new millennium 32, 544-583
- 10. Kenstowicz, Michael (2004): Verbal tone in Buli: a morphosyntactic analysis.: MIT
- 11. Kenstowicz, Michael & George Akanlig-Pare (2003): Studies in Buli Grammar: Working papers on endangered and less familiar languages. Cambridge, MA: Dept. of Linguistics, Massachusetts Inst. of Technology (MIT): MITWPL
- 12. Schwarz, Anne (2006): Sentence-medial adverbials in Buli.: Afrikanistentag München Handout
- 13. Schwarz, Anne (2009): To be or not to be? About the copula system in Buli (Gur). In: Margarida Petter & Ronald Beline Mendes (eds.), Proceedings of the Special World Congress of African Linguistics – São Paulo 2008: Exploring the African language conne. São Paulo: Humanitas, 263-278
- 14. Schwarz, Anne (2010): Discourse principles in grammar: the thetic/categorial dichotomy.:
- 15. Schwarz, Anne (2016): All-in-one and one-for-all: Thetic structures in Buli grammar and discourse. In: Doric L. Payne, Sara Pacchiarott and Mokaya Bosire (eds.) Diversity in African languages. Berlin: Language Science Press, 81-100